Hi, where are you from?

My photo
Presents, a Life with a Plan. My name is Karen Anastasia Placek, I am the author of this Google Blog. This is the story of my journey, a quest to understanding more than myself. The title of my first blog delivered more than a million views!! The title is its work as "The Secret of the Universe is Choice!; know decision" will be the next global slogan. Placed on T-shirts, Jackets, Sweatshirts, it really doesn't matter, 'cause a picture with my slogan is worth more than a thousand words, it's worth??.......Know Conversation!!!

Friday, October 19, 2018

Might A Singer Sing As A Funny Thing Happened And I Remembered To Forget Until. . . .



The average man will not understand the following:  The hippocampus is the holder of the human soul.  As that is now comprehended the soul is not owned.  Fact that the soul can be sold upon entry, such knowledge is not left to conjecture as the very thought to consideration sets the brain to the intended recipient.  This practical magic once, is now not even once upon a time to exit the garden and enter mankind, it is the logic that dictates the following to comprehend the continued.  Consider the 'Soul' a G.P.S. system and it navigates you home in the afterlife.

Loss of the hippocampus through lobotomy removes short-time memory as that would in logical steps return the mind to it's pre-birth natural state and the brain would still continue to function as a receptor of translation to the vocal authority of the owned, our brain is our responsibility to retain inside of our skull unless doctors decide to enter the cavity of our skull through our eyeball, at that point the resistance cannot be addressed as the formative would be changed and the result is obvious, change would dictate to the surroundings and be molded by both right and left in basic order to obtain good standing.

Memory would be ground zero as the remainder of what is a life would come in good stature.  The old ways of memory itself is aspect however barbarism is known to be the start-up through a procedure known as lobotomy.  To juice ones brain is in the extract, memory of such has already been put to the board at the posture of comfort and not a push to rush the know as that would have been destructive to myself.  As memory creates space as proven by computers it also is understood by simply studying computer technology that memory is just a matter of creation of more RAM, these are the facts of life easily verified by the computer itself.  Proof is in the technology as that is why applications appeal to society, easy games to spread messages, basic tic, tac, toe has been the extent of it so far.  Perhaps with more interest the Computer Technological Companies of Silicon Valley will bring those applications back to the drawing board and create some real programs. 

The hippocampus is a major component of the brains of humans and other vertebrates. Humans and other mammals have two hippocampi, one in each side of ...
Part of‎: ‎Temporal lobe

Hippocampus (disambiguation)


Hippocampus may refer to: Hippocampus, an anatomical ...

Brain


Pages in category "Hippocampus (brain)". The following 44 pages ...

Hippocampus anatomy


Hippocampus anatomy describes the physical aspects and ...

Dentate gyrus


The dentate gyrus is part of a brain region known as the ...
The hippocampus is part of the mammalian brain, and belongs to the limbic system. Humans and other mammals have two, one in each side of the brain.
The hippocampal formation is a compound structure in the medial temporal lobe of the brain. There is no consensus concerning which brain regions are ...
The hippocampus or hippocamp, also hippokampoi often called a sea-horse in English, is a mythological creature shared by Phoenician, Etruscan, and Greek ...

Hippocampus

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Jump to navigation Jump to search
Hippocampus
Gray739-emphasizing-hippocampus.png
The hippocampus is located in the medial temporal lobe of the brain. In this lateral view of the human brain, the frontal lobe is at the left, the occipital lobe at the right, and the temporal and parietal lobes have largely been removed to reveal the hippocampus underneath.
1511 The Limbic Lobe.jpg
Hippocampus (lowest pink bulb)
as part of the limbic system
Details
Part ofTemporal lobe
Identifiers
LatinHippocampus
NeuroLex IDbirnlex_721
TAA14.1.09.321
FMA275020
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy
The hippocampus (named after its resemblance to the seahorse, from the Greek ἱππόκαμπος, "seahorse" from ἵππος hippos, "horse" and κάμπος kampos, "sea monster") is a major component of the brains of humans and other vertebrates. Humans and other mammals have two hippocampi, one in each side of the brain. The hippocampus belongs to the limbic system and plays important roles in the consolidation of information from short-term memory to long-term memory, and in spatial memory that enables navigation. The hippocampus is located under the cerebral cortex (allocortical)[1][2][3] and in primates in the medial temporal lobe. It contains two main interlocking parts: the hippocampus proper (also called Ammon's horn)[4] and the dentate gyrus.
In Alzheimer's disease (and other forms of dementia), the hippocampus is one of the first regions of the brain to suffer damage; short-term memory loss and disorientation are included among the early symptoms. Damage to the hippocampus can also result from oxygen starvation (hypoxia), encephalitis, or medial temporal lobe epilepsy. People with extensive, bilateral hippocampal damage may experience anterograde amnesia (the inability to form and retain new memories).
In rodents as model organisms, the hippocampus has been studied extensively as part of a brain system responsible for spatial memory and navigation. Many neurons in the rat and mouse hippocampus respond as place cells: that is, they fire bursts of action potentials when the animal passes through a specific part of its environment. Hippocampal place cells interact extensively with head direction cells, whose activity acts as an inertial compass, and conjecturally with grid cells in the neighboring entorhinal cortex.
Since different neuronal cell types are neatly organized into layers in the hippocampus, it has frequently been used as a model system for studying neurophysiology. The form of neural plasticity known as long-term potentiation (LTP) was initially discovered to occur in the hippocampus and has often been studied in this structure. LTP is widely believed to be one of the main neural mechanisms by which memories are stored in the brain.

Name

Image 1: The human hippocampus and fornix compared with a seahorse[5]
The earliest description of the ridge running along the floor of the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle comes from the Venetian anatomist Julius Caesar Aranzi (1587), who likened it first to a silkworm and then to a seahorse (Latin: hippocampus from Greek: ἵππος, "horse" and κάμπος, "sea monster"). The German anatomist Duvernoy (1729), the first to illustrate the structure, also wavered between "seahorse" and "silkworm." "Ram's horn" was proposed by the Danish anatomist Jacob Winsløw in 1732; and a decade later his fellow Parisian, the surgeon de Garengeot, used "cornu Ammonis" – horn of (the ancient Egyptian god) Amun,[6] who was often represented as having a ram's head.[7] This has survived in abbreviated form as CA in naming the subfields of the hippocampus. Another reference appeared with the term pes hippocampi, which may date back to Diemerbroeck in 1672, introducing a comparison with the shape of the folded back forelimbs and webbed feet of the mythological hippocampus, a sea monster with a horse's forequarters and a fish's tail. The hippocampus was then described as pes hippocampi major, with an adjacent bulge in the occipital horn, described as the pes hippocampi minor and later renamed as the calcar avis.[6][8] The renaming of the hippocampus as hippocampus major, and the calcar avis as hippocampus minor, has been attributed to Félix Vicq-d'Azyr systematising nomenclature of parts of the brain in 1786. Mayer mistakenly used the term hippopotamus in 1779, and was followed by some other authors until Karl Friedrich Burdach resolved this error in 1829. In 1861 the hippocampus minor became the centre of a dispute over human evolution between Thomas Henry Huxley and Richard Owen, satirised as the Great Hippocampus Question. The term hippocampus minor fell from use in anatomy textbooks, and was officially removed in the Nomina Anatomica of 1895.[9] Today, the structure is just called the hippocampus,[6] with the term Cornu Ammonis surviving in the names of the hippocampal subfields CA1–CA4.[10]
The term limbic system was introduced in 1952 by Paul MacLean[11] to describe the set of structures that line the edge of the cortex (Latin limbus meaning border): These include the hippocampus, cingulate cortex, olfactory cortex, and amygdala. Paul MacLean later suggested that the limbic structures comprise the neural basis of emotion. The hippocampus is anatomically connected to parts of the brain that are involved with emotional behavior—the septum, the hypothalamic mammillary body, and the anterior nuclear complex in the thalamus, and is generally accepted to be part of the limbic system.[12]

Anatomy

Image 2: Cross-section of cerebral hemisphere showing structure and location of hippocampus
Image 3: Coronal section of the brain of a macaque monkey, showing hippocampus (circled)
The hippocampus can be seen as a ridge of gray matter tissue, elevating from the floor of each lateral ventricle in the region of the inferior or temporal horn.[13][14] This ridge can also be seen as an inward fold of the archicortex into the medial temporal lobe.[15] The hippocampus can only be seen in dissections as it is concealed by the parahippocampal gyrus.[15][16] The cortex thins from six layers to the three or four layers that make up the hippocampus.[17]
The term hippocampal formation is used to refer to the hippocampus proper and its related parts. However, there is no consensus as to what parts are included. Sometimes the hippocampus is said to include the dentate gyrus and the subiculum. Some references include the dentate gyrus and the subiculum in the hippocampal formation,[1] and others also include the presubiculum, parasubiculum, and entorhinal cortex.[2] The neural layout and pathways within the hippocampal formation are very similar in all mammals.[3]
The hippocampus, including the dentate gyrus, has the shape of a curved tube, which has been compared to a seahorse, and a ram's horn (Cornu Ammonis). Its abbreviation CA is used in naming the hippocampal subfields: CA1, CA2, CA3, and CA4.[16] It can be distinguished as an area where the cortex narrows into a single layer of densely packed pyramidal neurons, which curl into a tight U shape. One edge of the "U," – CA4, is embedded into the backward-facing, flexed dentate gyrus. The hippocampus is described as having an anterior and posterior part (in primates) or a ventral and dorsal part in other animals. Both parts are of similar composition but belong to different neural circuits.[18] In the rat, the two hippocampi resemble a pair of bananas, joined at the stems by the commissure of fornix (also called the hippocampal commissure). In primates, the part of the hippocampus at the bottom, near the base of the temporal lobe, is much broader than the part at the top. This means that in cross-section the hippocampus can show a number of different shapes, depending on the angle and location of the cut.
In a cross-section of the hippocampus, including the dentate gyrus, several layers will be shown. The dentate gyrus has three layers of cells (or four if the hilus is included). The layers are from the outer in - the molecular layer, the inner molecular layer, the granular layer, and the hilus. The CA3 in the hippocampus proper has the following cell layers known as strata: lacunosum-moleculare, radiatum, lucidum, pyramidal, and oriens. CA2 and CA1 also have four each of these layers and not the lucidum stratum.
The input to the hippocampus (from varying cortical and subcortical structures) comes from the entorhinal cortex via the perforant path. The entorhinal cortex (EC) is strongly and reciprocally connected with many cortical and subcortical structures as well as with the brainstem. Different thalamic nuclei, (from the anterior and midline groups), the medial septal nucleus, the supramamillary nucleus of the hypothalamus, and the raphe nuclei and locus coeruleus of the brainstem all send axons to the EC, so that it serves as the interface between the neocortex and the other connections, and the hippocampus.
The EC, is located in the parahippocampal gyrus,[2] a cortical region adjacent to the hippocampus.[19] This gyrus conceals the hippocampus. The parahippocampal gyrus also includes the perirhinal cortex which plays an important role in the visual recognition of complex objects. There is also substantial evidence that it makes a contribution to memory, which can be distinguished from the contribution of the hippocampus. It is apparent that complete amnesia occurs only when both the hippocampus and the parahippocampus are damaged.[19]

Circuitry

Image 4: Basic circuit of the hippocampus, as drawn by Cajal DG: dentate gyrus. Sub: subiculum. EC: entorhinal cortex.
The perforant path is the main output pathway of the entorhinal cortex (EC). The flow of information in the hippocampus is largely unidirectional. Axons from the pyramidal cells (in EC layer II) perforate the subiculum to project mainly to the granular layer in the dentate gyrus (first synaptic connection). Some axons project to CA3 and a lesser number project to CA1. The dentate granule cell axons, called mossy fibers, pass on the information, to the dendrites of CA3 pyramidal cells (second synaptic connection). From there, CA3 axons called Schaffer collaterals leave the deep part of the cell body and loop up to the apical dendrites and then extend to CA1. Axons from CA1 then project back to the entorhinal cortex (third synaptic connection) completing the trisynaptic loop or circuit.[20]
Basket cells in CA3 receive excitatory input from the pyramidal cells and then give an inhibitory feedback to the pyramidal cells. This recurrent inhibition is a simple feedback circuit that can dampen excitatory responses in the hippocampus. The pyramidal cells gives a recurrent excitation which is an important mechanism found in some memory processing microcircuits.[21]
Several other connections play important roles in hippocampal function.[16] Beyond the output to the EC, additional output pathways go to other cortical areas including the prefrontal cortex. A major output goes via the fornix to the lateral septal area and to the mammillary body of the hypothalamus (which the fornix interconnects with the hippocampus).[15] The hippocampus receives modulatory input from the serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine systems, and from the nucleus reuniens of the thalamus to field CA1. A very important projection comes from the medial septal nucleus, which sends cholinergic, and gamma amino butyric acid (GABA) stimulating fibers (GABAergic fibers) to all parts of the hippocampus. The inputs from the medial septal nucleus play a key role in controlling the physiological state of the hippocampus; destruction of this nucleus abolishes the hippocampal theta rhythm and severely impairs certain types of memory.[22]

Hippocampal regions

Image 5: Hippocampal location and regions
Areas of the hippocampus are shown to be functionally and anatomically distinct. The dorsal hippocampus (DH), ventral hippocampus (VH) and intermediate hippocampus serve different functions, project with differing pathways, and have varying degrees of place cells.[23] The dorsal hippocampus serves for spatial memory, verbal memory, and learning of conceptual information. Using the radial arm maze, lesions in the DH were shown to cause spatial memory impairment while VH lesions did not. Its projecting pathways include the medial septal nucleus and supramammillary nucleus.[24] The dorsal hippocampus also has more place cells than both the ventral and intermediate hippocampal regions.[25]
The intermediate hippocampus has overlapping characteristics with both the ventral and dorsal hippocampus.[23] Using anterograde tracing methods, Cenquizca and Swanson (2007) located the moderate projections to two primary olfactory cortical areas and prelimbic areas of the medial prefrontal cortex. This region has the smallest number of place cells. The ventral hippocampus functions in fear conditioning and affective processes.[26] Anagnostaras et al. (2002) showed that alterations to the ventral hippocampus reduced the amount of information sent to the amygdala by the dorsal and ventral hippocampus, consequently altering fear conditioning in rats.[27] Historically, the earliest widely held hypothesis was that the hippocampus is involved in olfaction.[28] This idea was cast into doubt by a series of anatomical studies that did not find any direct projections to the hippocampus from the olfactory bulb.[29] However, later work did confirm that the olfactory bulb does project into the ventral part of the lateral entorhinal cortex, and field CA1 in the ventral hippocampus sends axons to the main olfactory bulb,[30] the anterior olfactory nucleus, and to the primary olfactory cortex. There continues to be some interest in hippocampal olfactory responses, in particular, the role of the hippocampus in memory for odors, but few specialists today believe that olfaction is its primary function.[31][32]

Function

Theories of hippocampal functions

Over the years, three main ideas of hippocampal function have dominated the literature: response inhibition, episodic memory, and spatial cognition. The behavioral inhibition theory (caricatured by John O'Keefe and Lynn Nadel as "slam on the brakes!")[33] was very popular up to the 1960s. It derived much of its justification from two observations: first, that animals with hippocampal damage tend to be hyperactive; second, that animals with hippocampal damage often have difficulty learning to inhibit responses that they have previously been taught, especially if the response requires remaining quiet as in a passive avoidance test. Jeffrey Gray developed this line of thought into a full-fledged theory of the role of the hippocampus in anxiety.[34] The inhibition theory is currently the least popular of the three.[35]
The second major line of thought relates the hippocampus to memory. Although it had historical precursors, this idea derived its main impetus from a famous report by William Beecher Scoville and Brenda Milner[36] describing the results of surgical destruction of the hippocampi (in an attempt to relieve epileptic seizures), in Henry Molaison,[37] known until his death in 2008 as "Patient H.M." The unexpected outcome of the surgery was severe anterograde and partial retrograde amnesia; Molaison was unable to form new episodic memories after his surgery and could not remember any events that occurred just before his surgery, but he did retain memories of events that occurred many years earlier extending back into his childhood. This case attracted such widespread professional interest that Molaison became the most intensively studied subject in medical history.[38] In the ensuing years, other patients with similar levels of hippocampal damage and amnesia (caused by accident or disease) have also been studied, and thousands of experiments have studied the physiology of activity-driven changes in synaptic connections in the hippocampus. There is now universal agreement that the hippocampi play some sort of important role in memory; however, the precise nature of this role remains widely debated.[39][40]
The third important theory of hippocampal function relates the hippocampus to space. The spatial theory was originally championed by O'Keefe and Nadel, who were influenced by E.C. Tolman's theories about "cognitive maps" in humans and animals. O'Keefe and his student Dostrovsky in 1971 discovered neurons in the rat hippocampus that appeared to them to show activity related to the rat's location within its environment.[41] Despite skepticism from other investigators, O'Keefe and his co-workers, especially Lynn Nadel, continued to investigate this question, in a line of work that eventually led to their very influential 1978 book The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map.[42] There is now almost universal agreement that hippocampal function plays an important role in spatial coding, but the details are widely debated.[43]
Later research has focused on trying to bridge the disconnect between the two main views of hippocampal function as being split between memory and spatial cognition. In some studies, these areas have been expanded to the point of near convergence. In an attempt to reconcile the two disparate views it is suggested that a broader view of the hippocampal function is taken and seen to have a role that encompasses both the organisation of experience (mental mapping), (as per Tolman's original concept in 1948) and the directional behaviour seen as being involved in all areas of cognition. So that the function of the hippocampus can be viewed as a broader system that incorporates both the memory and the spatial perspectives in its role that involves the use of a wide scope of cognitive maps.[44] This relates to the purposive behaviorism born of Tolman's original goal of identifying the complex cognitive mechanisms and purposes that guided behaviour.[45]
It has also been proposed that the spiking activity of hippocampal neurons is associated spatially, and it was suggested that the mechanisms of memory and planning both evolved from mechanisms of navigation and that their neuronal algorithms were basically the same.[46]
Many studies have made use of neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a functional role in approach-avoidance conflict has been noted. The anterior hippocampus is seen to be involved in decision-making under approach-avoidance conflict processing. It is suggested that the memory, spatial cognition, and conflict processing functions may be seen as working together and not mutually exclusive.[47]

Role in memory

Psychologists and neuroscientists generally agree that the hippocampus plays an important role in the formation of new memories about experienced events (episodic or autobiographical memory).[40][48] Part of this function is hippocampal involvement in the detection of new events, places and stimuli.[49] Some researchers regard the hippocampus as part of a larger medial temporal lobe memory system responsible for general declarative memory (memories that can be explicitly verbalized—these would include, for example, memory for facts in addition to episodic memory).[39] The hippocampus also encodes emotional context from the amygdala. This is partly why returning to a location where an emotional event occurred may evoke that emotion. There is a deep emotional connection between episodic memories and places.[50]
Due to bilateral symmetry the brain has a hippocampus in each cerebral hemisphere. If damage to the hippocampus occurs in only one hemisphere, leaving the structure intact in the other hemisphere, the brain can retain near-normal memory functioning.[51] Severe damage to the hippocampi in both hemispheres results in profound difficulties in forming new memories (anterograde amnesia) and often also affects memories formed before the damage occurred (retrograde amnesia). Although the retrograde effect normally extends many years back before the brain damage, in some cases older memories remain. This retention of older memories leads to the idea that consolidation over time involves the transfer of memories out of the hippocampus to other parts of the brain.[52] Experiments using intrahippocampal transplantation of hippocampal cells in primates with neurotoxic lesions of the hippocampus have shown that the hippocampus is required for the formation and recall, but not the storage, of memories.[53]
Damage to the hippocampus does not affect some types of memory, such as the ability to learn new skills (playing a musical instrument or solving certain types of puzzles, for example). This fact suggests that such abilities depend on different types of memory (procedural memory) and different brain regions. Furthermore, amnesic patients frequently show "implicit" memory for experiences even in the absence of conscious knowledge. For example, patients asked to guess which of two faces they have seen most recently may give the correct answer most of the time in spite of stating that they have never seen either of the faces before. Some researchers distinguish between conscious recollection, which depends on the hippocampus, and familiarity, which depends on portions of the medial temporal lobe.[54]

Role in spatial memory and navigation

Image 6: Spatial firing patterns of 8 place cells recorded from the CA1 layer of a rat. The rat ran back and forth along an elevated track, stopping at each end to eat a small food reward. Dots indicate positions where action potentials were recorded, with color indicating which neuron emitted that action potential.
Studies on freely moving rats and mice have shown many hippocampal neurons to act as place cells that cluster in place fields, and these fire bursts of action potentials when the animal passes through a particular location. This place-related neural activity in the hippocampus has also been reported in monkeys that were moved around a room whilst in a restraint chair.[55] However, the place cells may have fired in relation to where the monkey was looking rather that to its actual location in the room.[56] Over many years, many studies have been carried out on place-responses in rodents, that have given a large amount of information.[43] Place cell responses are shown by pyramidal cells in the hippocampus and by granule cells in the dentate gyrus. Other cells in smaller proportion are inhibitory interneurons and these often show place-related variations in their firing rate which are much weaker. There is little if any spatial topography in the representation; in general, cells lying next to each other in the hippocampus have uncorrelated spatial firing patterns. Place cells are typically almost silent when a rat is moving around outside the place field but reach sustained rates as high as 40 Hz, when the rat is near the center. Neural activity sampled from 30 to 40 randomly chosen place cells carries enough information to allow a rat's location to be reconstructed with high confidence. The size of place fields varies in a gradient along the length of the hippocampus, with cells at the dorsal end showing the smallest fields, cells near the center showing larger fields, and cells at the ventral tip showing fields that cover the entire environment.[43] In some cases, the firing rate of hippocampal cells depends not only on place but also on the direction a rat is moving, the destination toward which it is traveling, or other task-related variables.[57] The firing of place cells is timed in relation to local theta waves, a process termed phase precession.[58]
In humans, cells with location-specific firing patterns have been reported during a study of patients with drug-resistant epilepsy. They were undergoing an invasive procedure to localize the source of their seizures, with a view to surgical resection. The patients had diagnostic electrodes implanted in their hippocampus and then used a computer to move around in a virtual reality town.[59] Similar brain imaging studies in navigation have shown the hippocampus to be active.[60] A study was carried out on taxi drivers. London’s black cab drivers need to learn the locations of a large number of places and the fastest routes between them in order to pass a strict test known as The Knowledge in order to gain a license to operate. A study showed that the posterior part of the hippocampus is larger in these drivers than in the general public, and that a positive correlation exists between the length of time served as a driver and the increase in the volume of this part. It was also found the total volume of the hippocampus was unchanged, as the increase seen in the posterior part was made at the expense of the anterior part, which showed a relative decrease in size. There have been no reported adverse effects from this disparity in hippocampal proportions.[61] Another study showed opposite findings in blind individuals. The anterior part of the right hippocampus was larger and the posterior part was smaller, comparing with sighted individuals.[62]
There are several navigational cells in the brain that are either in the hippocampus itself or are strongly connected to it, such as the speed cells present in the medial entorhinal cortex. Together these cells form a network that serves as spatial memory. The first of such cells discovered in the 1970s were the place cells, which led to the idea of the hippocampus acting to give a neural representation of the environment in a cognitive map.[63] When the hippocampus is dysfunctional, orientation is affected; people may have difficulty in remembering how they arrived at a location and how to proceed further. Getting lost is a common symptom of amnesia.[64] Studies with animals have shown that an intact hippocampus is required for initial learning and long-term retention of some spatial memory tasks, in particular ones that require finding the way to a hidden goal.[65][66][67][68] Other cells have been discovered since the finding of the place cells in the rodent brain that are either in the hippocampus or the entorhinal cortex. These have been assigned as head direction cells, grid cells and boundary cells.[43][69] Speed cells are thought to provide input to the hippocampal grid cells.

Role in approach-avoidance conflict processing

Approach-avoidance conflict happens when a situation is presented that can either be rewarding or punishing, and the ensuing decision-making has been associated with anxiety.[70] fMRI findings from studies in approach-avoidance decision-making found evidence for a functional role that is not explained by either long-term memory or spatial cognition. Overall findings showed that the anterior hippocampus is sensitive to conflict, and that it may be part of a larger cortical and subcortical network seen to be important in decision making in uncertain conditions.[70]
A review makes reference to a number of studies that show the involvement of the hippocampus in conflict tasks. The authors suggest that a challenge is to understand how conflict processing relates to the functions of spatial navigation and memory and how all of these functions need not be mutually exclusive.[47]

Physiology

Image 7: Examples of rat hippocampal EEG and CA1 neural activity in the theta (awake/behaving) and LIA (slow-wave sleep) modes. Each plot shows 20 seconds of data, with a hippocampal EEG trace at the top, spike rasters from 40 simultaneously recorded CA1 pyramidal cells in the middle (each raster line represents a different cell), and a plot of running speed at the bottom. The top plot represents a time period during which the rat was actively searching for scattered food pellets. For the bottom plot the rat was asleep.
The hippocampus shows two major "modes" of activity, each associated with a distinct pattern of neural population activity and waves of electrical activity as measured by an electroencephalogram (EEG). These modes are named after the EEG patterns associated with them: theta and large irregular activity (LIA). The main characteristics described below are for the rat, which is the animal most extensively studied.[71]
The theta mode appears during states of active, alert behavior (especially locomotion), and also during REM (dreaming) sleep.[72] In the theta mode, the EEG is dominated by large regular waves with a frequency range of 6 to 9 Hz, and the main groups of hippocampal neurons (pyramidal cells and granule cells) show sparse population activity, which means that in any short time interval, the great majority of cells are silent, while the small remaining fraction fire at relatively high rates, up to 50 spikes in one second for the most active of them. An active cell typically stays active for half a second to a few seconds. As the rat behaves, the active cells fall silent and new cells become active, but the overall percentage of active cells remains more or less constant. In many situations, cell activity is determined largely by the spatial location of the animal, but other behavioral variables also clearly influence it.
The LIA mode appears during slow-wave (non-dreaming) sleep, and also during states of waking immobility such as resting or eating.[72] In the LIA mode, the EEG is dominated by sharp waves that are randomly timed large deflections of the EEG signal lasting for 25–50 milliseconds. Sharp waves are frequently generated in sets, with sets containing up to 5 or more individual sharp waves and lasting up to 500 ms. The spiking activity of neurons within the hippocampus is highly correlated with sharp wave activity. Most neurons decrease their firing rate between sharp waves; however, during a sharp wave, there is a dramatic increase in firing rate in up to 10% of the hippocampal population
These two hippocampal activity modes can be seen in primates as well as rats, with the exception that it has been difficult to see robust theta rhythmicity in the primate hippocampus. There are, however, qualitatively similar sharp waves and similar state-dependent changes in neural population activity.[73]

Theta rhythm

Image 8: Example of a one-second EEG theta wave
Because of its densely packed neural layers, of all the brain structures which generate the hippocampal theta rhythm, the hippocampus generates some of the largest EEG signals as theta waves .[74] In some situations the EEG is dominated by regular waves at 3 to 10 Hz, often continuing for many seconds. These reflect subthreshold membrane potentials and strongly modulate the spiking of hippocampal neurons and synchronise across the hippocampus in a travelling wave pattern.[75] The trisynaptic circuit is a relay of neurotransmission in the hippocampus that interacts with many brain regions. From rodent studies it has been proposed that the trisynaptic circuit generates the hippocampal theta rhythm.[76]
Theta rhythmicity is very obvious in rabbits and rodents and also clearly present in cats and dogs. Whether theta can be seen in primates is not yet clear.[77] In rats (the animals that have been the most extensively studied), theta is seen mainly in two conditions: first, when an animal is walking or in some other way actively interacting with its surroundings; second, during REM sleep.[78] The function of theta has not yet been convincingly explained although numerous theories have been proposed.[71] The most popular hypothesis has been to relate it to learning and memory. An example would be the phase with which theta rhythms, at the time of stimulation of a neuron, shape the effect of that stimulation upon its synapses. What is meant here is that theta rhythms may affect those aspects of learning and memory that are dependent upon synaptic plasticity.[79] It is well established that lesions of the medial septum—the central node of the theta system—cause severe disruptions of memory. However, the medial septum is more than just the controller of theta; it is also the main source of cholinergic projections to the hippocampus.[16] It has not been established that septal lesions exert their effects specifically by eliminating the theta rhythm.[80]

Sharp waves

During sleep or during resting, when an animal is not engaged with its surroundings, the hippocampal EEG shows a pattern of irregular slow waves, somewhat larger in amplitude than theta waves. This pattern is occasionally interrupted by large surges called sharp waves.[81] These events are associated with bursts of spike activity lasting 50 to 100 milliseconds in pyramidal cells of CA3 and CA1. They are also associated with short-lived high-frequency EEG oscillations called "ripples", with frequencies in the range 150 to 200 Hz in rats, and together they are known as sharp waves and ripples. Sharp waves are most frequent during sleep when they occur at an average rate of around 1 per second (in rats) but in a very irregular temporal pattern. Sharp waves are less frequent during inactive waking states and are usually smaller. Sharp waves have also been observed in humans and monkeys. In macaques, sharp waves are robust but do not occur as frequently as in rats.[73]
One of the most interesting aspects of sharp waves is that they appear to be associated with memory. Wilson and McNaughton 1994,[82] and numerous later studies, reported that when hippocampal place cells have overlapping spatial firing fields (and therefore often fire in near-simultaneity), they tend to show correlated activity during sleep following the behavioral session. This enhancement of correlation, commonly known as reactivation, has been found to occur mainly during sharp waves.[83] It has been proposed that sharp waves are, in fact, reactivations of neural activity patterns that were memorized during behavior, driven by strengthening of synaptic connections within the hippocampus.[84] This idea forms a key component of the "two-stage memory" theory, advocated by Buzsáki and others, which proposes that memories are stored within the hippocampus during behavior and then later transferred to the neocortex during sleep. Sharp waves in Hebbian theory are seen as persistently repeated stimulations by presynaptic cells, of postsynaptic cells that are suggested to drive synaptic changes in the cortical targets of hippocampal output pathways.[85] Suppression of sharp waves and ripples in sleep or during immobility can interfere with memories expressed at the level of the behavior,[86][87] nonetheless, the newly formed CA1 place cell code can re-emerge even after a sleep with abolished sharp waves and ripples, in spatially non-demanding tasks.[88]

Long-term potentiation

Since at least the time of Ramon y Cajal (1852-1934), psychologists have speculated that the brain stores memory by altering the strength of connections between neurons that are simultaneously active.[89] This idea was formalized by Donald Hebb in 1949,[90] but for many years remained unexplained. In 1973, Tim Bliss and Terje Lømo described a phenomenon in the rabbit hippocampus that appeared to meet Hebb's specifications: a change in synaptic responsiveness induced by brief strong activation and lasting for hours or days or longer.[91] This phenomenon was soon referred to as long-term potentiation (LTP). As a candidate mechanism for long-term memory, LTP has since been studied intensively, and a great deal has been learned about it. However, the complexity and variety of the intracellular signalling cascades that can trigger LTP is acknowledged as preventing a more complete understanding.[92]
The hippocampus is a particularly favorable site for studying LTP because of its densely packed and sharply defined layers of neurons, but similar types of activity-dependent synaptic change have also been observed in many other brain areas.[93] The best-studied form of LTP has been seen in CA1 of the hippocampus and occurs at synapses that terminate on dendritic spines and use the neurotransmitter glutamate.[92] The synaptic changes depend on a special type of glutamate receptor, the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor, a cell surface receptor which has the special property of allowing calcium to enter the postsynaptic spine only when presynaptic activation and postsynaptic depolarization occur at the same time.[94] Drugs that interfere with NMDA receptors block LTP and have major effects on some types of memory, especially spatial memory. Genetically modified mice that are modified to disable the LTP mechanism, also generally show severe memory deficits.[94]

Pathology

Aging

Age-related conditions such as Alzheimer's disease and other forms of dementia (for which hippocampal disruption is one of the earliest signs[95]) have a severe impact on many types of cognition, but even normal aging is associated with a gradual decline in some types of memory, including episodic memory and working memory (or short-term memory). Because the hippocampus is thought to play a central role in memory, there has been considerable interest in the possibility that age-related declines could be caused by hippocampal deterioration.[96] Some early studies reported substantial loss of neurons in the hippocampus of elderly people, but later studies using more precise techniques found only minimal differences.[96] Similarly, some MRI studies have reported shrinkage of the hippocampus in elderly people, but other studies have failed to reproduce this finding. There is, however, a reliable relationship between the size of the hippocampus and memory performance; so that where there is age-related shrinkage, memory performance will be impaired.[97] There are also reports that memory tasks tend to produce less hippocampal activation in the elderly than in the young.[97] Furthermore, a randomized control trial published in 2011 found that aerobic exercise could increase the size of the hippocampus in adults aged 55 to 80 and also improve spatial memory.[98]

Effects of cortisol

The hippocampus contains high levels of glucocorticoid receptors, which make it more vulnerable to long-term stress than most other brain areas.[99] There is evidence that humans having experienced severe, long-lasting traumatic stress show atrophy of the hippocampus more than of other parts of the brain.[100] These effects show up in post-traumatic stress disorder,[101] and they may contribute to the hippocampal atrophy reported in schizophrenia[102] and severe depression.[103] A recent study has also revealed atrophy as a result of depression, but this can be stopped with anti-depressants even if they are not effective in relieving other symptoms.[104]
Chronic stress resulting in elevated levels of glucocorticoids, notably of cortisol, is seen to be a cause of neuronal atrophy in the hippocampus.This atrophy results in a smaller hippocampal volume which is also seen in Cushing’s syndrome. The higher levels of cortisol in Cushing’s syndrome is usually the result of medications taken for other conditions.[105][106] Neuronal loss also occurs as a result of impaired neurogenesis. Another factor that contributes to a smaller hippocampal volume is that of dendritic retraction where dendrites are shortened in length and reduced in number, in response to increased glucocorticoids. This dendritic retraction is reversible.[106] After treatment with medication to reduce cortisol in Cushing’s syndrome, the hippocampal volume is seen to be restored by as much as 10%.[105] This change is seen to be due to the reforming of the dendrites.[106] This dendritic restoration can also happen when stress is removed. There is, however, evidence derived mainly from studies using rats that stress occurring shortly after birth can affect hippocampal function in ways that persist throughout life.[107]
Sex-specific responses to stress have also been demonstrated in the rat to have an effect on the hippocampus. Chronic stress in the male rat showed dendritic retraction and cell loss in the CA3 region but this was not shown in the female. This was thought to be due to neuroprotective ovarian hormones.[108][109]

Epilepsy

Image 10: An EEG showing epilepsy right-hippocampal seizure onset
Image 9: An EEG showing epilepsy left-hippocampal seizure onset
The hippocampus is one of the few brain regions where new neurons are generated. This process of neurogenesis is confined to the dentate gyrus.[110] The production of new neurons can be positively affected by exercise or negatively affected by epileptic seizures.[111]
Seizures in temporal lobe epilepsy can affect the normal development of new neurons and can cause tissue damage. Hippocampal sclerosis is the most common type of such tissue damage.[112] It is not yet clear, however, whether the epilepsy is usually caused by hippocampal abnormalities or whether the hippocampus is damaged by cumulative effects of seizures.[113] However, in experimental settings where repetitive seizures are artificially induced in animals, hippocampal damage is a frequent result. This may be a consequence of the concentration of excitable glutamate receptors in the hippocampus. Hyperexcitability can lead to cytotoxicity and cell death.[106] It may also have something to do with the hippocampus being a site where new neurons continue to be created throughout life,[114] and to abnormalities in this process.[106]

Schizophrenia

The causes of schizophrenia are not well understood, but numerous abnormalities of brain structure have been reported. The most thoroughly investigated alterations involve the cerebral cortex, but effects on the hippocampus have also been described. Many reports have found reductions in the size of the hippocampus in schizophrenic subjects.[115][116] The left hippocampus seems to be affected more than the right.[115] The changes noted have largely been accepted to be the result of abnormal development. It is unclear whether hippocampal alterations play any role in causing the psychotic symptoms that are the most important feature of schizophrenia. It has been suggested that on the basis of experimental work using animals, hippocampal dysfunction might produce an alteration of dopamine release in the basal ganglia, thereby indirectly affecting the integration of information in the prefrontal cortex.[117] It has also been suggested that hippocampal dysfunction might account for the disturbances in long-term memory frequently observed.[118]
MRI studies have found a smaller brain volume and larger ventricles in people with schizophrenia–however researchers do not know if the shrinkage is from the schizophrenia or from the medication.[119][120] The hippocampus and thalamus have been shown to be reduced in volume; and the volume of the globus pallidus is increased. Cortical patterns are altered, and a reduction in the volume and thickness of the cortex particularly in the frontal and temporal lobes has been noted. It has further been proposed that many of the changes seen are present at the start of the disorder which gives weight to the theory that there is abnormal neurodevelopment.[121]
The hippocampus has been seen as central to the pathology of schizophrenia, both in the neural and physiological effects.[115] It has been generally accepted that there is an abnormal synaptic connectivity underlying schizophrenia. Several lines of evidence implicate changes in the synaptic organization and connectivity, in and from the hippocampus[115] Many studies have found dysfunction in the synaptic circuitry within the hippocampus and its activity on the prefrontal cortex. The glutamatergic pathways have been seen to be largely affected. The subfield CA1 is seen to be the least involved of the other subfields,[115][122] and CA4 and the subiculum have been reported elsewhere as being the most implicated areas.[122] The review concluded that the pathology could be due to genetics, faulty neurodevelopment or abnormal neural plasticity. It was further concluded that schizophrenia is not due to any known neurodegenerative disorder.[115]

Transient global amnesia

Transient global amnesia is a dramatic, sudden, temporary, near-total loss of short-term memory. Various causes have been hypothesized including ischemia, epilepsy, migraine[123] and disturbance of cerebral venous blood flow,[124] leading to ischemia of structures such as the hippocampus that are involved in memory.[125]
There has been no scientific proof of any cause. However, diffusion weighted MRI studies taken from 12–24 hours following an episode has shown there to be small dot-like lesions in the hippocampus. These findings have suggested a possible implication of CA1 neurons made vulnerable by metabolic stress.[123]

PTSD

Some studies shows correlation of reduced hippocampus volume and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).[126][127][128] A study of Vietnam War combat veterans with PTSD showed a 20% reduction in the volume of their hippocampus compared with veterans having suffered no such symptoms.[129] This finding was not replicated in chronic PTSD patients traumatized at an air show plane crash in 1988 (Ramstein, Germany).[130] A 2016 study strengthened theory that a smaller hippocampus increases the risk for post-traumatic stress disorder, and a larger hippocampus increases the likelihood of efficacious treatment.[131]

Other animals

Image 11: Drawing by Camillo Golgi of a hippocampus stained using the silver nitrate method
The hippocampus has a generally similar appearance across the range of mammals, from monotremes such as the echidna to primates such as humans.[132] The hippocampal-size-to-body-size ratio broadly increases, being about twice as large for primates as for the echidna. It does not, however, increase at anywhere close to the rate of the neocortex-to-body-size ratio. Therefore, the hippocampus takes up a much larger fraction of the cortical mantle in rodents than in primates. In adult humans the volume of the hippocampus on each side of the brain is about 3.0 to 3.5 cm3 as compared to 320 to 420 cm3 for the volume of the neocortex.[133]
There is also a general relationship between the size of the hippocampus and spatial memory. When comparisons are made between similar species, those that have a greater capacity for spatial memory tend to have larger hippocampal volumes.[134] This relationship also extends to sex differences; in species where males and females show strong differences in spatial memory ability they also tend to show corresponding differences in hippocampal volume.[135]
Non-mammalian species do not have a brain structure that looks like the mammalian hippocampus, but they have one that is considered homologous to it. The hippocampus, as pointed out above, is in essence part of the allocortex. Only mammals have a fully developed cortex, but the structure it evolved from, called the pallium, is present in all vertebrates, even the most primitive ones such as the lamprey or hagfish.[136] The pallium is usually divided into three zones: medial, lateral and dorsal. The medial pallium forms the precursor of the hippocampus. It does not resemble the hippocampus visually because the layers are not warped into an S shape or enfolded by the dentate gyrus, but the homology is indicated by strong chemical and functional affinities. There is now evidence that these hippocampal-like structures are involved in spatial cognition in birds, reptiles, and fish.[137]
In birds, the correspondence is sufficiently well established that most anatomists refer to the medial pallial zone as the "avian hippocampus".[138] Numerous species of birds have strong spatial skills, in particular those that cache food. There is evidence that food-caching birds have a larger hippocampus than other types of birds and that damage to the hippocampus causes impairments in spatial memory.[139]
The story for fish is more complex. In teleost fish (which make up the great majority of existing species), the forebrain is distorted in comparison to other types of vertebrates: Most neuroanatomists believe that the teleost forebrain is in essence everted, like a sock turned inside-out, so that structures that lie in the interior, next to the ventricles, for most vertebrates, are found on the outside in teleost fish, and vice versa.[140] One of the consequences of this is that the medial pallium ("hippocampal" zone) of a typical vertebrate is thought to correspond to the lateral pallium of a typical fish. Several types of fish (particularly goldfish) have been shown experimentally to have strong spatial memory abilities, even forming "cognitive maps" of the areas they inhabit.[134] There is evidence that damage to the lateral pallium impairs spatial memory.[141][142]
It is not yet known whether the medial pallium plays a similar role in even more primitive vertebrates, such as sharks and rays, or even lampreys and hagfish. Some types of insects, and molluscs such as the octopus, also have strong spatial learning and navigation abilities, but these appear to work differently from the mammalian spatial system, so there is as yet no good reason to think that they have a common evolutionary origin; nor is there sufficient similarity in brain structure to enable anything resembling a "hippocampus" to be identified in these species. Some have proposed, however, that the insect's mushroom bodies may have a function similar to that of the hippocampus.[143]

Additional images

See also

Notes

References


  • Martin, JH (2003). "Lymbic system and cerebral circuits for emotions, learning, and memory". Neuroanatomy: text and atlas (third ed.). McGraw-Hill Companies. p. 382. ISBN 978-0-07-121237-3.

  • References

    d*Buzsáki G (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-530106-9.

    Further reading

    Journals

    Books

    • Anderson P, Morris R, Amaral, Bliss T, O'Keefe J, eds. (2007). The Hippocampus Book. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-510027-3.
    • Derdikman D, Knierim JJ, eds. (2014). Space, Time and Memory in the Hippocampal Formation. Springer. ISBN 978-3-7091-1292-2.
    • Duvernoy HM, Cattin F (2005). The Human Hippocampus: Functional Anatomy, Vascularization, and Serial Sections with MRI. Springer. ISBN 978-3-540-23191-2.
    • Eichenbaum H (2002). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory. Oxford University Press US. ISBN 978-0-19-514175-7.
    • Sharp PE, ed. (2002). The Neural Basis of Navigation: Evidence from Single Cell Recording. Springer. ISBN 978-0-7923-7579-1.
    • Taupin P (2007). The Hippocampus: Neurotransmission and Plasticity in the Nervous System. Nova Publishers. ISBN 978-1-60021-914-6.
    • Byrne JH, ed. (2008). Learning and Memory: A comprehensive reference. Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-12-370509-9.

    External links

    Navigation menu

    Languages



  • Amaral D, Lavenex P (2007). "Hippocampal neuroanatomy". In Anderson P, Morris R, Amaral, Bliss T, O'Keefe J. The hippocampus book (first ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. p. 37. ISBN 978-0-19-510027-3.

  • Anderson P, Morris R, Amaral, Bliss T, O'Keefe J (2007). "The hippocampal formation". In Anderson P, Morris R, Amaral, Bliss T, O'Keefe J. The hippocampus book (first ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. p. 3. ISBN 978-0-19-510027-3.

  • Pearce, 2001

  • preparation by László Seress in 1980.

  • Duvernoy, 2005

  • "cornu ammonis". TheFreeDictionary.com.

  • Owen CM, Howard A, Binder DK (December 2009). "Hippocampus minor, calcar avis, and the Huxley-Owen debate". Neurosurgery. 65 (6): 1098–104, discussion 1104–5. doi:10.1227/01.neu.0000359535.84445.0b. PMID 19934969.

  • Gross, 1993

  • Wechsler, 2004

  • Roxo MR, Franceschini PR, Zubaran C, Kleber FD, Sander JW (2011). "The limbic system conception and its historical evolution". TheScientificWorldJournal. 11: 2428–41. doi:10.1100/2011/157150. PMC 3236374. PMID 22194673.

  • "Chapter 9: Limbic System". www.dartmouth.edu.

  • Andersen P, Morris R, Amaral D, Bliss T, O'Keefe J (2 November 2006). "The Hippocampus Book". Oxford University Press.

  • Alberts, Daniel Albert (2012). Dorland's illustrated medical dictionary (32nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders/Elsevier. p. 860. ISBN 978-1-4160-6257-8.

  • Purves D (2011). Neuroscience (5th ed.). Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. pp. 730–735. ISBN 978-0-87893-695-3.

  • Amaral and Lavenex, 2006

  • Purves, Dale (2011). Neuroscience (5. ed.). Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. p. 590. ISBN 978-087893-695-3.

  • Moser and Moser, 1998

  • Eichenbaum et al, 2007

  • Purves, Dale (2011). Neuroscience (5th ed.). Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. p. 171. ISBN 978-0-87893-695-3.

  • "Introduction to Neurons and Neuronal Networks | Section 1, Intro Chapter | Neuroscience Online: An Electronic Textbook for the Neurosciences | Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy - The University of Texas Medical School at Houston". neuroscience.uth.tmc.edu. Archived from the original on 2013-12-03.

  • Winson, 1978

  • Fanselow, 2010

  • Pothuizen et al., 2004

  • Jung et al., 1994

  • Cenquizca et al., 2007

  • Anagnostaras et al., 2002

  • Finger, S (2001). "Defining and controlling the circuits of emotion". Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function. Oxford/NewYork: Oxford University Press. p. 286. ISBN 978-0-19-506503-9.

  • Finger, p. 183

  • "Extrinsic projections from area CA1 of the rat hippocampus: olfactory, cortical, subcortical, and bilateral hippocampal formation projections". Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990. doi:10.1002/cne.903020308.

  • Eichenbaum et al, 1991

  • Vanderwolf, 2001

  • Nadel et al., 1975

  • Gray and McNaughton, 2000

  • Best & White, 1999

  • Scoville and Milner, 1957

  • New York Times, 12-06-2008

  • Squire, 2009

  • Squire, 1992

  • Eichenbaum and Cohen, 1993

  • O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971

  • O'Keefe and Nadel, 1978

  • Moser et al., 2008

  • Schiller D, Eichenbaum H, Buffalo EA, Davachi L, Foster DJ, Leutgeb S, Ranganath C (October 2015). "Memory and Space: Towards an Understanding of the Cognitive Map". The Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (41): 13904–11. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2618-15.2015. PMID 26468191.

  • Eichenbaum H (2001). "The hippocampus and declarative memory: Cognitive mechanisms and neural codes". Behavioural Brain Research. 127 (1): 199–207. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00365-5.

  • Buzsáki G, Moser EI (February 2013). "Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system". Nature Neuroscience. 16 (2): 130–8. doi:10.1038/nn.3304. PMC 4079500. PMID 23354386.

  • Ito R, Lee AC (October 2016). "The role of the hippocampus in approach-avoidance conflict decision-making: Evidence from rodent and human studies". Behavioural Brain Research. 313: 345–57. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2016.07.039. PMID 27457133.

  • Squire and Schacter, 2002

  • VanElzakker et al., 2008

  • Gluck M, Mercado E, Myers C (2014). Learning and Memory From Brain to Behavior Second Edition. New York: Kevin Feyen. p. 416. ISBN 978-1-4292-4014-7.

  • Di Gennaro G, Grammaldo LG, Quarato PP, Esposito V, Mascia A, Sparano A, Meldolesi GN, Picardi A (Jun 2006). "Severe amnesia following bilateral medial temporal lobe damage occurring on two distinct occasions". Neurological Sciences. 27 (2): 129–33. doi:10.1007/s10072-006-0614-y. PMID 16816912.

  • Squire and Schacter, 2002, Ch. 1

  • Virley D, Ridley RM, Sinden JD, Kershaw TR, Harland S, Rashid T, French S, Sowinski P, Gray JA, Lantos PL, Hodges H (December 1999). "Primary CA1 and conditionally immortal MHP36 cell grafts restore conditional discrimination learning and recall in marmosets after excitotoxic lesions of the hippocampal CA1 field". Brain : A Journal of Neurology. 122 ( Pt 12) (12): 2321–35. doi:10.1093/brain/122.12.2321. PMID 10581225.

  • Diana et al., 2007

  • Matsumura et al., 1999

  • Rolls and Xiang, 2006

  • Smith and Mizumori, 2006

  • O'Keefe J, Recce ML (July 1993). "Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm". Hippocampus. 3 (3): 317–30. doi:10.1002/hipo.450030307. PMID 8353611.

  • Ekstrom et al., 2003

  • Duarte IC, Ferreira C, Marques J, Castelo-Branco M (2014-01-27). "Anterior/posterior competitive deactivation/activation dichotomy in the human hippocampus as revealed by a 3D navigation task". PLOS One. 9 (1): e86213. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...986213D. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086213. PMC 3903506. PMID 24475088.

  • Maguire et al., 2000

  • Leporé N, Shi Y, Lepore F, Fortin M, Voss P, Chou YY, Lord C, Lassonde M, Dinov ID, Toga AW, Thompson PM (July 2009). "Pattern of hippocampal shape and volume differences in blind subjects". NeuroImage. 46 (4): 949–57. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.071. PMC 2736880. PMID 19285559.

  • O'Keefe and Nadel

  • Chiu et al., 2004

  • Morris et al., 1982

  • Sutherland et al., 1982

  • Sutherland et al., 2001

  • Clark et al., 2005

  • Solstad et al., 2008

  • O'Neil EB, Newsome RN, Li IH, Thavabalasingam S, Ito R, Lee AC (November 2015). "Examining the Role of the Human Hippocampus in Approach-Avoidance Decision Making Using a Novel Conflict Paradigm and Multivariate Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging". The Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (45): 15039–49. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1915-15.2015. PMID 26558775.

  • Buzsáki, 2006

  • Buzsáki et al., 1990

  • Skaggs et al., 2007

  • Buzsáki, 2002

  • Lubenov & Siapas, 2009

  • Komisaruk, B. R. (1970). "Synchrony between limbic system theta activity and rhythmical behavior in rats". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 70 (3): 482.

  • Cantero et al., 2003

  • Vanderwolf, 1969

  • Huerta & Lisman, 1993

  • Kahana et al., 2001

  • Buzsáki, 1986

  • Wilson & McNaughton, 1994

  • Jackson et al., 2006

  • Sutherland & McNaughton, 2000

  • Buzsáki, 1989

  • Girardeau G, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Buzsáki G, Zugaro MB (October 2009). "Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory". Nature Neuroscience. 12 (10): 1222–3. doi:10.1038/nn.2384. PMID 19749750.

  • Ego-Stengel V, Wilson MA (January 2010). "Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat". Hippocampus. 20 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1002/hipo.20707. PMC 2801761. PMID 19816984.

  • Kovacs KA, O'Neill J, Schoenenberger P, Penttonen M, Ranguel Guerrero DK, Csicsvari J (19 Nov 2016). "Optogenetically Blocking Sharp Wave Ripple Events in Sleep Does Not Interfere with the Formation of Stable Spatial Representation in the CA1 Area of the Hippocampus". PLOS One. 11 (10): e0164675. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1164675K. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0164675. PMC 5070819. PMID 27760158.

  • Ramon y Cajal, 1894

  • Hebb, 1949

  • Bliss & Lømo, 1973

  • Malenka & Bear, 2004

  • Cooke & Bliss, 2006

  • Nakazawa et al., 2004

  • Hampel et al., 2008

  • Prull et al., 2000, p. 105

  • Prull et al., 2000, p. 107

  • Erickson et al., 2011

  • Joels, 2008

  • Fu et al, 2010

  • Karl A, Schaefer M, Malta LS, Dörfel D, Rohleder N, Werner A (2006). "A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 30 (7): 1004–31. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.03.004. PMID 16730374.

  • Wright IC, Rabe-Hesketh S, Woodruff PW, David AS, Murray RM, Bullmore ET (January 2000). "Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 157 (1): 16–25. doi:10.1176/ajp.157.1.16. PMID 10618008.

  • Kempton MJ, Salvador Z, Munafò MR, Geddes JR, Simmons A, Frangou S, Williams SC (Jul 2011). "Structural neuroimaging studies in major depressive disorder. Meta-analysis and comparison with bipolar disorder". Archives of General Psychiatry. 68 (7): 675–90. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.60. PMID 21727252. see also MRI database at www.depressiondatabase.org

  • Campbell & MacQueen, 2004

  • Starkman MN, Giordani B, Gebarski SS, Berent S, Schork MA, Schteingart DE (December 1999). "Decrease in cortisol reverses human hippocampal atrophy following treatment of Cushing's disease". Biological Psychiatry. 46 (12): 1595–602. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00203-6. PMID 10624540.

  • Disorders, Institute of Medicine (US) Forum on Neuroscience and Nervous System (1 January 2011). "Overview of the Glutamatergic System". National Academies Press (US).

  • Garcia-Segura, pp. 170–71

  • Conrad CD (2008). "Chronic stress-induced hippocampal vulnerability: the glucocorticoid vulnerability hypothesis". Reviews in the Neurosciences. 19 (6): 395–411. doi:10.1515/revneuro.2008.19.6.395. PMC 2746750. PMID 19317179.

  • Ortiz JB, McLaughlin KJ, Hamilton GF, Baran SE, Campbell AN, Conrad CD (August 2013). "Cholesterol and perhaps estradiol protect against corticosterone-induced hippocampal CA3 dendritic retraction in gonadectomized female and male rats". Neuroscience. 246: 409–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.04.027. PMC 3703463. PMID 23618757.

  • Kuruba et al., 2009

  • Kuruba et al., 2009

  • Chang and Lowenstein, 2003

  • Sloviter, 2005

  • Kuruba et al., 2009

  • Harrison, 2004

  • Antoniadesa, M., Schoelera, T., Raduaa, J., Vallia, I., Allena, P., Kemptona, M. J., McGuirea, P., (2017). Verbal learning and hippocampal dysfunction in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 1(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.001

  • Goto & Grace, 2008

  • Boyer et al., 2007

  • Ho BC, Andreasen NC, Ziebell S, Pierson R, Magnotta V (February 2011). "Long-term antipsychotic treatment and brain volumes: a longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia". Archives of General Psychiatry. 68 (2): 128–37. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.199. PMC 3476840. PMID 21300943.

  • Fusar-Poli P, Smieskova R, Kempton MJ, Ho BC, Andreasen NC, Borgwardt S (September 2013). "Progressive brain changes in schizophrenia related to antipsychotic treatment? A meta-analysis of longitudinal MRI studies". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 37 (8): 1680–91. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.001. PMC 3964856. PMID 23769814.

  • Haukvik UK, Hartberg CB, Agartz I (April 2013). "Schizophrenia--what does structural MRI show?". Tidsskrift for den Norske Laegeforening. 133 (8): 850–3. doi:10.4045/tidsskr.12.1084. PMID 23612107.

  • Harrison PJ, Eastwood SL (2001). "Neuropathological studies of synaptic connectivity in the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia". Hippocampus. 11 (5): 508–19. doi:10.1002/hipo.1067. PMID 11732704.

  • Szabo K (2014). "Transient global amnesia". Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience. 34: 143–9. doi:10.1159/000356431. ISBN 978-3-318-02567-5. PMID 24777137.

  • Lewis SL (August 1998). "Aetiology of transient global amnesia". Lancet. 352 (9125): 397–9. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(98)01442-1. PMID 9717945.

  • Chung CP, Hsu HY, Chao AC, Chang FC, Sheng WY, Hu HH (June 2006). "Detection of intracranial venous reflux in patients of transient global amnesia". Neurology. 66 (12): 1873–7. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000219620.69618.9d. PMID 16801653.

  • Bonne O, Vythilingam M, Inagaki M, Wood S, Neumeister A, Nugent AC, Snow J, Luckenbaugh DA, Bain EE, Drevets WC, Charney DS (July 2008). "Reduced posterior hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 69 (7): 1087–91. doi:10.4088/jcp.v69n0707. PMC 2684983. PMID 18572983.

  • Apfel BA, Ross J, Hlavin J, Meyerhoff DJ, Metzler TJ, Marmar CR, Weiner MW, Schuff N, Neylan TC (March 2011). "Hippocampal volume differences in Gulf War veterans with current versus lifetime posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms". Biological Psychiatry. 69 (6): 541–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.044. PMC 3259803. PMID 21094937.

  • "Hippocampal volume and resilience in posttramatic stress disorder". ScienceDaily. 23 March 2011.

  • Carlson, Neil R. (2014). Physiology of Behavior (11 ed.). Pearson Education. p. 624. ISBN 978-1-292-02320-5.

  • Jatzko A, Rothenhöfer S, Schmitt A, Gaser C, Demirakca T, Weber-Fahr W, Wessa M, Magnotta V, Braus DF (August 2006). "Hippocampal volume in chronic posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): MRI study using two different evaluation methods" (PDF). Journal of Affective Disorders. 94 (1–3): 121–6. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.010. PMID 16701903.

  • Rubin M, Shvil E, Papini S, Chhetry BT, Helpman L, Markowitz JC, Mann JJ, Neria Y (June 2016). "Greater hippocampal volume is associated with PTSD treatment response". Psychiatry Research. Neuroimaging. 252: 36–39. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.05.001. PMC 4896219. PMID 27179314.

  • West, 1990

  • Suzuki et al, 2005

  • Jacobs, 2003

  • Jacobs et al., 1990

  • Aboitiz et al., 2003

  • Rodríguez et al., 2002

  • Colombo and Broadbent, 2000

  • Shettleworth, 2003

  • Nieuwenhuys, 1982

  • Portavella et al., 2002

  • Vargas et al., 2006
  •  

  • Hippocampus (mythology)

    From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
    Jump to navigation Jump to search
    Winged hippocamp in an Art Deco fountain, Kansas City, Missouri, (1937)
    The hippocampus or hippocamp, also hippokampoi (plural: hippocampi or hippocamps; Greek: ἱππόκαμπος, from ἵππος, "horse" and κάμπος, "sea monster"[1]), often called a sea-horse[2] in English, is a mythological creature shared by Phoenician[3], Etruscan, and Greek mythology, though its name has a Greek origin. The hippocampus has typically been depicted as having the upper body of a horse with the lower body of a fish.

    Mythology

    Coins minted at Tyre around the 4th century BC show the patron god Melqart riding on a winged hippocampus and accompanied by dolphins.[3] Coins of the same period from Byblos show a hippocampus diving under a galley.[4][5]
    A gold sea-horse was discovered in a hoard from the kingdom of Lydia in Asia minor, dating to the 6th century BC.[6]

    Greek and Roman

    Hippocampus in Roman mosaic in the thermae at Aquae Sulis (Bath)
    In the Iliad, Homer describes Poseidon, god of horses, earthquakes, and the sea, drawn by brazen-hoofed horses over the sea's surface,[7] and Apollonius of Rhodes, describes the horse of Poseidon emerging from the sea and galloping across the Libyan sands.[8] This compares to the specifically "two-hoofed" hippocampi of Gaius Valerius Flaccus in his Argonautica: "Orion when grasping his father’s reins heaves the sea with the snorting of his two-hooved horses."[9] In Hellenistic and Roman imagery, however, Poseidon (or Roman Neptune) often drives a sea-chariot drawn by hippocampi. Thus hippocampi sport with this god in both ancient depictions and much more modern ones, such as in the waters of the 18th-century Trevi Fountain in Rome surveyed by Neptune from his niche above.
    The appearance of hippocampi in both freshwater and saltwater is counter-intuitive to a modern audience, though not to an ancient one. The Greek picture of the natural hydrological cycle did not take into account the condensation of atmospheric water as rain to replenish the water table, but imagined the waters of the sea oozing back landwards through vast underground caverns and aquifers, rising replenished and freshened in springs.[10]
    Tritons and winged hippocampi in the Trevi Fountain, Rome
    Thus it was natural for a temple at Helike in the coastal plain of Achaea to be dedicated to Poseidon Helikonios, (the Poseidon of Helicon), the sacred spring of Boeotian Helikon.[11] When an earthquake suddenly submerged the city, the temple's bronze Poseidon accompanied by hippocampi continued to snag fishermens' nets.[12] Likewise, the hippocampus was considered an appropriate decoration for mosaics in Roman thermae or public baths, as at Aquae Sulis modern day Bath in Britannia (illustration, below).
    Poseidon's horses, which were included in the elaborate sculptural program of gilt-bronze and ivory, added by a Roman client to the temple of Poseidon at Corinth, are likely to have been hippocampi; the Romanised Greek Pausanias described the rich ensemble in the later 2nd century AD (Geography of Greece ii.1.7-.8):
    On the temple, which is not very large, stand bronze Tritons. In the fore-temple are images, two of Poseidon, a third of Amphitrite, and a Sea, which also is of bronze. The offerings inside were dedicated in our time by Herodes Atticus, four horses, gilded except for the hoofs, which are of ivory, and two gold Tritons beside the horses, with the parts below the waist of ivory. On the car stand Amphitrite and Poseidon, and there is the boy Palaemon upright upon a dolphin. These too are made of ivory and gold. On the middle of the base on which the car has been wrought a Sea holding up the young Aphrodite, and on either side are the nymphs called Nereids.

    Etruscan

    Hippocampi appear with the first Oriental-phase of Etruscan civilization: they remain a theme in Etruscan tomb wall-paintings and reliefs,[13] where they are sometimes provided with wings, as they are in the Trevi fountain. Katharine Shepard found in the theme an Etruscan belief in a sea-voyage to the other world.[14]

    Pictish

    The sea-horse also appears in Pictish stone carvings in Scotland. The symbolism of the carving (also known as "Pictish Beast") is unknown. Although similar but not identical to Roman sea-horse images, it is unclear whether this depiction originates from images brought over by the Romans, or had a place in earlier Pictish mythology.[15]

    Medieval and Renaissance, and Modern

    The "sea-horse" in medieval heraldry was a legendary creature that was part horse and part fish, not to be confused with the later heraldic hippocampus, which was a natural seahorse.
    Hippocampuses in Dublin, Ireland.
    The mythic hippocampus has been used as a heraldic charge, particularly since the Renaissance, most often in the armorial bearings of people and places with maritime associations. However, in a blazon, the terms hippocamp and hippocampus now refer to the real animal called a seahorse, and the terms seahorse and sea-horse refer to the mythological creature. The above-mentioned fish hybrids are seen less frequently.[16]
    The sea-horse is also a common image in Renaissance and post-renaissance art, for example, in the Trevi fountain, dating to 1732.
    A winged hippocampus has been used as a symbol for Air France since its establishment in 1933 (inherited from its predecessor Air Orient); it appears today on the engine nacelles of Air France aircraft.
    Bronze hippocampoi appear in Dublin, Leinster, Ireland on lampposts next to a statue of Henry Grattan and on Grattan Bridge.

    Capricornus and related mythical animals

    Closely related to the hippocampus is the "sea goat", represented by Capricorn, a mythical creature with the front half of a goat and the rear half of a fish. Canonical figures, most of which were not themselves cult images, and coins of the Carian goddess associated with Aphrodite as the Aphrodite of Aphrodisias through interpretatio graeca, show the goddess riding on a sea-goat.[17] Brody describes her thus:[18]
    ... a semi-nude female figure appears riding on a sea-goat, accompanied by a dolphin and a Triton. This is the goddess Aphrodite herself, shown here not in her distinctive local guise but in a more traditionally Hellenistic style. She is the marine aspect of Aphrodite, known to the Greeks as Aphrodite Pelagia .... She rides on a fantastic marine creature with the body and tail of a fish and the forepart of a goat. This sea-goat moves to the right and turns his head back to look at the goddess. This group also appears on Aphrodisian coins from the 3rd century A.D.
    Aside from aigikampoi, the fish-tailed goats representing Capricorn,[17] other fish-tailed animals rarely appeared in Greek art, but are more characteristic of the Etruscans. These include leokampoi (fish-tailed lions), taurokampoi (fish-tailed bulls) or pardalokampoi (fish-tailed leopards).[19]

    See also

    References

    Notes



  • Word origin of Hippocampus at reference.com; compare the nameless monster Campe.

    1. Ippokampoi at Theoi Project (retrieved Jan. 11, 2013); see also Booson 1986.

    Sources

    External links

    Navigation menu

    Languages



  • The hyphen distinguishes from the seahorse, a real fish.

  • Israel Antiquities Authority, Yizre'el Valley silver hoard (retrieved Jan 10 2013)

  • Stater of Byblos with galley | Museum of Fine Arts, Boston

  • Byblos at NumisBids

  • Sharon Waxman, Loot: The Battle over the Stolen Treasures of the Ancient World, Chapter 6; excerpt in Smithsonian, Nov. 14, 2008 (retrieved Jan 10 2013).

  • Homer, Iliad xlii. 24, 29;

  • Apollonius of Rhodes, Argonautica (iv.1353ff)

  • Valerius Flaccus, Argonautica 2.507.

  • This made credible the mythic undersea passage of the fountain nymph Arethusa from Greece to Sicily. The summary given of the ancients' view of the hydrological cycle is outlined by the Roman Epicurean Lucretius' De rerum natura (vi.631-38).

  • Strabo: "The sea was raised by an earthquake and it submerged Helike and also the temple of Poseidon Helikonios..." (Geography 8.7.2).

  • According to Eratosthenes, noted by Strabo (loc. cit.).

  • Etruscan sea creatures, including a range of hippocampi, are set in cultural context and ordered by typology in Monika Boosen, Etruskische Meeresmischwesen: Untersuchungen zur Typologie u. Bedeutung (Archaeologica 59) (Rome:Bretschneider) 1986.

  • Katharine Shepard, The Fish-Tailed Monster in Greek and Etruscan Art, 1940, pp 25ff; the thesis was, exceptionally, reviewed (by G.W. Elderkin) in American Journal of Archaeology 45.2 (April 1941), pp. 307-308: available on-line through JSTOR.

  • Nigella Hillgarth, Birch Aquarium at Scripps Institute of Oceanography, in Myth, Legend & Art[permanent dead link], January 8, 2010 (retrieved January 10, 2013)

  • Fox-Davies, Arthur Charles. Complete Guide to Heraldry, 1978.

  • Imhoof-Blümer, Kleinasiatische Müntzen plate IV, no 14, noted in Elderkin 1941:307

  • Lisa R. Brody, under the direction of Christopher Ratté, "The Iconography and Cult of the Aphrodite of Aphrodisias" (dead link- archive version here), New York University, Institute of Fine Arts, 1999. (google books link)




  • No comments:

    Post a Comment

    An Independent Mind, Knot Logic

    An Independent Mind, Knot Logic

    The Number Is Bigger Than The Word Stars In A Camp

      Camp Cantore Arithmetic is able to state that a complication of what is seen on T.V. is a guide to quote where is that end quote.  Cantore...

    Karen A. Placek, aka Karen Placek, K.A.P., KAP

    My photo
    Presents, a Life with a Plan. My name is Karen Anastasia Placek, I am the author of this Google Blog. This is the story of my journey, a quest to understanding more than myself. The title of my first blog delivered more than a million views!! The title is its work as "The Secret of the Universe is Choice!; know decision" will be the next global slogan. Placed on T-shirts, Jackets, Sweatshirts, it really doesn't matter, 'cause a picture with my slogan is worth more than a thousand words, it's worth??.......Know Conversation!!!

    Know Decision of the Public: Popular Posts!!